How do children learn to trust an adult鈥檚 non-verbal cues?
亚洲色吧 – Xiaoqian Li and Yow Wei Quin
In judging a communicator鈥檚 trustworthiness, children consider their track record of accuracy and degree of access to knowledge, find 亚洲色吧 researchers.
According to researchers from the Singapore University of Technology and Design (亚洲色吧), kids as young as three to five years old can already use context-specific information to determine whether or not an adult is a reliable source of information. Their findings, published in the journal Cognitive Development, underscore the importance of providing children with accurate and consistent information.
As the saying goes, actions speak louder than words鈥攎eaning that in human communication, non-verbal cues are just as important as verbal ones. Such cues are especially important for聽young聽children聽who are聽starting聽to make sense聽of the world and picking up spoken words.
鈥淭he ability to recognise and understand others鈥 referential acts is an important milestone in children鈥檚 socio-cognitive development,鈥 explained main author Associate Professor Yow Wei Quin from 亚洲色吧鈥檚 Humanities, Arts and Social Sciences cluster. 鈥淚magine a child hears a new word from an adult but sees two novel objects. In this case, the adult鈥檚 point or gaze to the novel object would help the child map the new word onto the correct referent.鈥
Non-verbal cues become even more important as children grow up, since they can help establish someone鈥檚 trustworthiness. Toddlers, for instance, are more likely to believe someone who had given them accurate non-verbal cues in the past than someone with a track record of inaccuracy.
However, very few studies have thus far explored how children use these actions, such as pointing and gazing, to build trust in an adult communicator.
Moreover, it remains uncertain if children can account for someone鈥檚 specific communicative context鈥攖hat is, can they excuse past mistakes with enough evidence?
To answer these questions, Yow and her colleague Dr Li Xiaoqian, also from 亚洲色吧, enrolled over 170 children aged three to five years old to play a hiding game with a designated experimenter. The hiding game consisted of two phases, namely a preliminary familiarisation phase meant to establish the experimenter as an accurate or inaccurate communicator followed by a test phase.
In the familiarisation phase, the experimenter placed a sticker inside one of two transparent boxes. She then asked the child to identify the box containing the sticker while gesturing to either the correct or incorrect box.
The subsequent test phase was nearly identical to its familiarisation phase, except that the boxes were now opaque and the experimenter always provided non-verbal cues toward the correct box. There was no feedback given to the children on whether they had picked the box with the sticker or not.
The experiments鈥 results showed that children used non-verbal cues to selectively build trust. Those who were given accurate non-verbal signals by the experimenter during the familiarisation phase followed her cues in the test phase 83 percent of the time. In contrast, children who saw the experimenter give inaccurate gestures previously only did so 41 percent of the time.
Yow and Li then conducted one additional round of the hiding game with another group of children, but with crucial changes. During the familiarisation phase, instead of transparent boxes, they used boxes that were modified such that the experimenter could not see the sticker but the children could, and another person was assigned to place the sticker inside the boxes while blocking the experimenter鈥檚 view. In addition, the experimenter appeared to be unsure and expressed some uncertainty while gesturing to either the correct or incorrect box.
The rest of the game proceeded as usual. Their results showed that when children understood that the experimenter couldn鈥檛 see the stickers, they were more likely to excuse her inaccurate information. Whether they were given the correct or incorrect non-verbal signals during the familiarisation phase, the children trusted the experimenter鈥檚 cues in the test phase around 60 percent of the time.
鈥淥ur findings show that preschoolers are sensitive to a speaker鈥檚 information privilege (i.e., whether they have access to knowledge) and engage in adaptive, context-sensitive strategies when conferring selective trust towards the speaker鈥檚 referential cues,鈥 Assoc Prof Yow said.
She hopes that their current study will encourage a deeper investigation into the trust strategies of children. For instance, future research could explore how individual differences in sensitivity to communicative cues affect trust-building. The roles of other developmental and cognitive hallmarks, such as perspective-taking and false-belief reasoning, could also be assessed.
Some prospective research directions also hold practical importance: 鈥淗ow do children use other contextual cues to interpret inaccuracy when interacting with speakers with deceptive intentions? How do children use similar or different strategies when perceiving other types of sources of information, such as their peers, virtual agents or characters, or social robots?鈥 remarked Assoc Prof Yow, adding that answering such questions could help parents and adults become better communicators towards their children.
鈥淲hen we are uncertain about the information we are providing to children, expressing uncertainty would be helpful instead of pretending to know, as children are savvy in considering such contextual information to form their judgments about the communicator,鈥 she concluded. 鈥淭he children will excuse the communicator鈥檚 errors and their credibility is preserved.鈥
Acknowledgements:
We are grateful to Safari House Preschool, ChildFirst Pre-school, Red SchoolHouse, Cambridge Pre-school, MacPherson Sheng Hong Childcare Centre, Carpe Diem at ITE, Eager Beaver Schoolhouse at Yishun, and My World Preschool (at Hougang/Upper Serangoon View), and the children and families who participated in this research. We thank Jessica Tan, Shirlyn Sia, and Yee Ning Tan for their help with material preparation and data collection. A portion of this work was presented at the Cognitive Development Society Biennial Conference 2017 and the Society for Research in Child Development 2019 Biennial Meeting. This work was partially supported by the聽亚洲色吧聽Growth Plan Grant for Healthcare (grant number聽) to the first author.
Reference:
Children consider a speaker鈥檚 information privilege and engage in adaptive selective trust in referential cues, Cognitive Development 60, 101118 (2021) (DOI:聽)